Subscribe now

Life

Blind Mexican cave fish are developing cave-specific accents

The Mexican tetra has evolved to live in a number of dark caves – and now we know that the fish in each cave use clicks to communicate in distinct ways

By Richard Kemeny

14 April 2022

blind cave tetra

The blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus) is developing unique accents in different caves

blickwinkel/A. Hartl/Alamy

In the underground caves of north-eastern Mexico, groups of blind fish appear to be developing cave-specific accents. The linguistic split could eventually contribute to ongoing speciation among the fish.

The Mexican tetra (Astyanax mexicanus) is no stranger to diversification. It exists in two forms: one with good vision that lives in light-drenched rivers, the other blind with a translucent body, which began evolving perhaps only 20,000 years ago as some fish populated dark underground caves.

Like many fish, A. mexicanus uses noise to communicate. It produces at least six distinct sounds for this, though their meaning seems to have shifted among the cave-dwellers as they adjusted to living in darkness. A particular form of sharp click used by sighted fish in aggressive encounters, for example, is produced by their blind counterparts while foraging.

Carole Hyacinthe at Harvard University wondered if the communication also varied between fish evolving in different caves.

Hyacinthe and her colleagues analysed 44 hours of fish chatter recorded in six caves, spread across the three mountain ranges where cave colonisation is thought to have taken place independently. The team focused on clicks and repeated clicks, the two most commonly used sounds. They compared a range of acoustic values, including the length of each click, pitch and the rate at which multiple clicks were produced in sequence.

They found several significant, distinct variations between the caves. Clicks were pitched relatively high in a cave called Molino, while they were deep and booming in a cave called Subterráneo. Fish inhabiting a cave called Pachón fired off clicks up to 10 times more rapidly than in other caves, while in a cave called Tinaja, clicks were more drawn out. In a cave called Chica, where hybrid populations of surface and cavefish are thought to live, sounds were more varied than elsewhere.

Sounds in a cave called Los Sabinos were similar to nearby Chica, but also Subterráneo and Molino, which are in different regions. This supports the idea that the sounds evolved independently and weren’t related to physical proximity.

The team then used a statistical analysis commonly deployed in acoustics to assign sounds into groups, which also suggested separate patterns in each cave.

“This is a very intriguing and creative piece of work,” says Suzanne McGaugh at the University of Minnesota, who wasn’t involved in the study. It would be interesting to see if communication varies between surface populations as well, she says.

The new accents are probably the result of totally random genetic drift, says Sylvie Rétaux at the University of Paris-Saclay, a co-author of the study.

Eventually, the authors speculate, communication difficulties could contribute to speciation. Further experiments involving sound playback would be critical to explore this. “Maybe after a million years it will have drifted so much that they will not be able to understand each other anymore,” says Rétaux.

Hyacinthe is now exploring sound production in the cavefish on a more fundamental level, as the mechanisms are still unknown. “Astyanax is a very good model to investigate the genetic basis of the evolution of sounds,” she says.

Reference: bioRxiv, DOI: 10.1101/2022.03.29.486255v1

Sign up to Wild Wild Life, a free monthly newsletter celebrating the diversity and science of animals, plants and Earth’s other weird and wonderful inhabitants

Topics:

Sign up to our weekly newsletter

Receive a weekly dose of discovery in your inbox! We'll also keep you up to date with New Scientist events and special offers.

Sign up