Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Four core properties of the human brain valuation system demonstrated in intracranial signals

Abstract

Estimating the value of alternative options is a key process in decision-making. Human functional magnetic resonance imaging and monkey electrophysiology studies have identified brain regions, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC), composing a value system. In the present study, in an effort to bridge across species and techniques, we investigated the neural representation of value ratings in 36 people with epilepsy, using intracranial electroencephalography. We found that subjective value was positively reflected in both vmPFC and lOFC high-frequency activity, plus several other brain regions, including the hippocampus. We then demonstrated that subjective value could be decoded (1) in pre-stimulus activity, (2) for various categories of items, (3) even during a distractive task and (4) as both linear and quadratic signals (encoding both value and confidence). Thus, our findings specify key functional properties of neural value signals (anticipation, generality, automaticity, quadraticity), which might provide insights into human irrational choice behaviors.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Behavioral tasks and results.
Fig. 2: Anatomical locations of recording sites in the whole dataset.
Fig. 3: Time–frequency investigation of the BVS-evoked response.
Fig. 4: Anticipation of value signaling during the pre-stimulus period in the BVS.
Fig. 5: Generality of value signaling in the BVS.
Fig. 6: Automaticity of value signaling in the BVS.
Fig. 7: Quadraticity of value signaling in the BVS.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author (A.L.-P.) upon request.

Code availability

The custom code used to generate the figures and statistics are available from the corresponding author (A.L.-P.) upon request.

References

  1. Krajbich, I., Armel, C. & Rangel, A. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1292–1298 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Hunt, L. T. et al. Mechanisms underlying cortical activity during value-guided choice. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 470–S3 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. O’Doherty, J. P. The problem with value. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 259–268 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.027 (2014).

  4. Juechems, K. & Summerfield, C. Where does value come from? Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 836–850 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Lopez-Persem, A., Rigoux, L., Bourgeois-Gironde, S., Daunizeau, J. & Pessiglione, M. Choose, rate or squeeze: comparison of economic value functions elicited by different behavioral tasks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13, e1005848 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Rangel, A., Camerer, C. & Montague, P. R. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 545–556 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T. & Kable, J. W. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage 76, 412–427 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Peters, J. & Büchel, C. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66, 138–148 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lebreton, M. et al. A critical role for the hippocampus in the valuation of imagined outcomes. PLoS Biol. 11, e1001684 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R. & Buckner, R. L. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 657–661 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Barron, H. C., Dolan, R. J. & Behrens, T. E. J. Online evaluation of novel choices by simultaneous representation of multiple memories. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1492–1498 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J. & Andrews, C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 95–102 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Zhang, Z. et al. Distributed neural representation of saliency controlled value and category during anticipation of rewards and punishments. Nat. Commun. 8, 1907 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Bouret, S. & Richmond, B. J. Ventromedial and orbital prefrontal neurons differentially encode internally and externally driven motivational values in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 30, 8591–8601 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Strait, C. E., Blanchard, T. C. & Hayden, B. Y. Reward value comparison via mutual inhibition in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 82, 1357–1366 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Padoa-Schioppa, C. Orbitofrontal cortex and the computation of economic value. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1121, 232–253 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tremblay, L. & Schultz, W. Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 398, 705–708 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Saez, I. et al. Encoding of multiple reward-related computations in transient and sustained high-frequency activity in human OFC. Curr. Biol. 28, 2889–2899 (2018).

  19. Abitbol, R. et al. Neural mechanisms underlying contextual dependency of subjective values: converging evidence from monkeys and humans. J. Neurosci. 35, 2308–2320 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Vinckier, F., Rigoux, L., Oudiette, D. & Pessiglione, M. Neuro-computational account of how mood fluctuations arise and affect decision making. Nat. Commun. 9, 1708 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Chib, V. S., Rangel, A., Shimojo, S. & O’Doherty, J. P. Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 12315–12320 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lebreton, M., Jorge, S., Michel, V., Thirion, B. & Pessiglione, M. An automatic valuation system in the human brain: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuron 64, 431–439 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Levy, D. J. & Glimcher, P. W. The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 1027–1038 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Levy, I., Lazzaro, S. C., Rutledge, R. B. & Glimcher, P. W. Choice from non-choice: predicting consumer preferences from blood oxygenation level-dependent signals obtained during passive viewing. J. Neurosci. 31, 118–125 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. De Martino, B., Fleming, S. M., Garrett, N. & Dolan, R. J. Confidence in value-based choice. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 105–110 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Lebreton, M., Abitbol, R., Daunizeau, J. & Pessiglione, M. Automatic integration of confidence in the brain valuation signal. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1159–1167 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage 15, 273–289 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. & Oeltermann, A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Scheeringa, R. et al. Neuronal dynamics underlying high- and low-frequency EEG oscillations contribute independently to the human BOLD signal. Neuron 69, 572–583 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Ray, S., Crone, N. E., Niebur, E., Franaszczuk, P. J. & Hsiao, S. S. Neural correlates of high-gamma oscillations (60–200 Hz) in macaque local field potentials and their potential implications in electrocorticography. J. Neurosci. 28, 11526–11536 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Bastin, J. et al. Direct recordings in human cortex reveal the dynamics of gamma-band [50–150 Hz] activity during pursuit eye movement control. NeuroImage 63, 339–347 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Jerbi, K. et al. Task-related gamma-band dynamics from an intracerebral perspective: review and implications for surface EEG and MEG. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1758–1771 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Lachaux, J.-P., Axmacher, N., Mormann, F., Halgren, E. & Crone, N. E. High-frequency neural activity and human cognition: Past, present and possible future of intracranial EEG research. Prog. Neurobiol. 98, 279–301 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Holm, S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand. J. Stat. 6, 65–70 (1979).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Wallis, J. D. & Kennerley, S. W. Heterogeneous reward signals in prefrontal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 191–198 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Hunt, L. T. & Hayden, B. Y. A distributed, hierarchical and recurrent framework for reward-based choice. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 172–182 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Buzsáki, G., Anastassiou, C. A. & Koch, C. The origin of extracellular fields and currents—EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 407–420 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Harvey, A. H., Kirk, U., Denfield, G. H. & Montague, P. R. Monetary favors and their influence on neural responses and revealed preference. J. Neurosci. 30, 9597–9602 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F. & Rangel, A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646–648 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kable, J. W. & Glimcher, P. W. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1625–1633 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Watson, K. K. & Platt, M. L. Social signals in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Curr. Biol. 22, 2268–2273 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Azzi, J. C. B., Sirigu, A. & Duhamel, J.-R. Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 2126–2131 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Grueschow, M., Polania, R., Hare, T. A. & Ruff, C. C. Automatic versus choice-dependent value representations in the human brain. Neuron 85, 874–885 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Grabenhorst, F. & Rolls, E. T. Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 723–729 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Mainen, Z. F. & Kepecs, A. Neural representation of behavioral outcomes in the orbitofrontal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 84–91 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Lachaux, J. P., Rudrauf, D. & Kahane, P. Intracranial EEG and human brain mapping. J. Physiol. Paris 97, 613–628 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging (Thieme Medical Publishers, 1988).

  48. Perez-Garcia, F. et al. Automatic segmentation of depth electrodes implanted in epileptic patients: a modular tool adaptable to multicentric protocols. Epilepsia 56, 227 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E. & Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011, https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869 (2011).

  50. Rigoux, L., Stephan, K. E., Friston, K. J. & Daunizeau, J. Bayesian model selection for group studies—revisited. NeuroImage 84, 971–985 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Stephan, K. E., Penny, W. D., Daunizeau, J., Moran, R. J. & Friston, K. J. Bayesian model selection for group studies. NeuroImage 46, 1004–1017 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bastin, J. et al. Direct recordings from human anterior insula reveal its leading role within the error-monitoring network. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1545–1557 (2016).

  53. Maris, E. & Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J.-P. Lachaux, M. El Zein, S. Bouret and E. Procyk for their helpful comments on the analysis of iEEG data, and Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière for purchasing the acquisition material. This work benefited from the program ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ (ANR-10-IAIHU-06), from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under grant agreement no. 720270 (HBP SGA1) and no. 785907 (HBP SGA2), from the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ (ANR-11-IDEX-0007), from IHU CESAME, within the program ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ (ANR-10-IBHU-0003), and from University Grenoble Alpes, within the program ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ (ANR-17-CE37-0018 and ANR-18-CE28-0016). A.L-P. received a PhD fellowship from Direction Générale de l’Armement and a grant from LabEx Bio-Psy. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M. Pessiglione designed all the experiments. A.L.-P., J.B., M. Petton and R.A. collected the data. J.B. provided preprocessing scripts. A.L.-P. performed the data analysis. C.A., V.N., S.R. and P.K. did the intracranial investigation and allowed the collection of iEEG data. K.L. supervised the access to patients in Paris. P.D. provided tools for the time–frequency analysis. A.L-P. and M. Pessiglione wrote the manuscript. All the authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Alizée Lopez-Persem or Mathias Pessiglione.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Distribution of ratings.

The different plots show distributions across rating types (first-order and second-order), tasks (age and likeability rating), stimuli (food, faces and paintings). Different colors correspond to different patients. Age ratings were restricted to the range 20-50 years old for faces and to the dates 1400-2000 for paintings.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Distribution of response times.

Two times were considered: response onset (first key press made to move the cursor and response validation (press of the validation key). Note that response onset for confidence rating was not recorded. The different plots show distributions across rating types (first-order and second-order), tasks (age and likeability rating), stimuli (food, faces and paintings). Different colors correspond to different patients.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Anatomical locations of vmPFC, lOFC, hippocampus and PHC recording sites in the whole and restricted datasets.

Top. Axial (top) and sagittal (bottom) slices of a brain template on which were superimposed the approximate locations of vmPFC (red), lOFC (blue), hippocampus (green) and PHC (brown) recording sites in all patients. Each dot represents one recording site. Bottom. Axial (top) and sagittal (bottom) slices of a brain template on which were superimposed the approximate locations of vmPFC, lOFC, hippocampus and PHC recording sites in patients who completed the long version of the task. Each dot represents one recording site. Different colors correspond to different patients.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Anatomical dissociation of the vmPFC and lOFC.

Left: Coronal view of the BVS, formed by the lOFC (blue) and the vmPFC (red). Right: Regression estimates of gamma and high gamma binned in function of the x-MNI location of each recording site. Dots (and error bars) indicate mean (and SEM) across n = 450 power time series. Solid curves represent a second-order polynomial fit across regression estimates, applied separately to each subpart of the BVS. Regression estimates varied with laterality (x-MNI coordinate), with a maximum at the center of each ROI (around x = −10 and 10 for vmPFC, and x=−30 and 30 for lOFC), supporting the anatomical position of the boundaries between ROIs.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Time-frequency investigation of the evoked response in supplementary BVS regions.

a, Anatomical localization of the inferior temporal cortex, fusiform anterior gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus and opercular part of the inferior frontal gyrus (one line per region). All recording sites located in those (bilateral) areas were included in the ROI analysis. N indicates the number of recording sites in each ROI. b, Time-frequency analysis of the evoked response following visual item onset (dashed vertical line) averaged over all recording sites and all food likeability rating trials. Hotter colors indicate higher power. Horizontal dashed lines indicate boundaries between frequency bands that are investigated in panels c and d. c, Regression estimates of power against food likeability rating, averaged over the 0.5-1 s window, for each frequency band defined in b. Center lines, center circles, box limits, whiskers and points of the box plots respectively represent median, mean, interquartile range, extreme data points and outliers of the data distribution. Black stars indicate significance of regression estimates (two-sided one-sample t-test, p<0.05). d, Time course of regression estimates for the gamma and high-gamma frequency band. Stars indicate significant regression estimate (two-sided one-sample t-test, p<0.05, uncorrected) at each time point. Shaded areas highlight the time window of interest used in c and in following analyses. n indicates the number of power time series included in the analysis. Solid (dashed) lines indicates mean (SEM) across power time series.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Decoding food likeability rating from high-frequency OFC (top, purple) and (P)HC (bottom, yellow) activity.

Left: Time course of decoding accuracy from OFC (top, pooling vmPFC and lOFC) and (P)HC (bottom, pooling hippocampus and PHC) signals. Solid lines represent mean accuracy across 100 testing folds from 225 (and 201) recording sites in the OFC (and (P)HC) in the high frequencies (gamma and high gamma) and dashed lines represent SEM (across tested folds). The accuracy of the classifier was tested against the accuracy of a classifier trained on shuffled ratings (grey line). Stars indicate significant difference (two-sided one-sample t-test, p<0.05, cluster-wise corrected across time points). Right: Regions of interest used in each time course. Red: vmPFC, blue: lOFC, green: hippocampus, brown: PHC.

Extended Data Fig. 7 High-frequency activity in the BVS sorted by likeability rating tertiles (high and low).

High-gamma activity in each ROI (vmPFC in red, lOFC in blue, hippocampus in green and PHC in orange/brown) was averaged separately for first (low, dark) and third (high, light) tertiles of likeability ratings, during both food likeability rating task and non-food age rating task. Solid lines indicate mean and dashed lines indicate SEM across all trials, independently of patients and recording sites. nt indicates the number of power time series (trials) included in the analysis.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Linear and quadratic links with food likeability in the BVS.

BVS: comparison between short and long versions of the task, and between squared rating and confidence judgment. a,b, Time courses of regression estimates obtained for linear (top) and squared (bottom) food likeability, locked on stimulus onset (left) or first button press (right), during the short (a) or long (b) version of the task. c, Time courses of regression estimates for squared first-order judgment (top) and confidence on these judgments (bottom), locked on stimulus onset (left) or first button press (right). First-order judgments mean age and likeability ratings pooled together. Solid lines indicate mean and dashed lines indicate SEM across recording sites. Stars indicate significant regression estimate (two-sided one-sample t-test, p<0.05, uncorrected) at each time point. Statistics: a. (post-stimulus: OFC: t(323)=4.43, p=1.10-5, (P)HC: t(255)=4.54, p=9.10-6; pre-response: OFC: t(323)=5.36, p=2.10-7, (P)HC: t(255)=6.43, p=6.10-10; two-sided one-sample t-tests). c. Regression against actual confidence judgments in OFC: squared first-order ratings, post-stimulus: t(125)=5.87, p=4.10-8; pre-response: t(125)=5.88, p=3.10-8; confidence ratings, post-stimulus: t(125)=5.05, p=1.10-6 ; pre-response: t(125)=3.57, p=5.10-4.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1 and 2.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Tables 1–8

Contains eight tabs with eight supplementary tables cited in the main text.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lopez-Persem, A., Bastin, J., Petton, M. et al. Four core properties of the human brain valuation system demonstrated in intracranial signals. Nat Neurosci 23, 664–675 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0615-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0615-9

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing