Introduction

Cultural evolution theory posits that a major factor in human ecological success is our high-fidelity and selective social learning, which permits the accumulation of valuable knowledge and skills over successive generations. One of the most cited types of selective social learning in the cultural evolution literature is to copy the behaviours of individuals highly respected and admired in a social group (i.e., prestigious individuals), known as prestige bias. Almost 20 years ago, Henrich and Gil-White (henceforth H&GW) developed a theory of the evolution of prestige. The distinctive features of this theory were the consideration of prestige as an alternative route to dominance to attain and maintain high social rank in humans and the relevance attributed to social learning in the evolution of prestige (Henrich and Gil-White, 2001). In this article, we focus on a crucial aspect of H&GW’s theory: the adaptive value and actual use of prestige-biased social learning in humans. In the following, we first outline H&GW’s theory. Second, we analyse whether prestige is associated with competence within valuable domains and older age. Third, we discuss which types of first-order (e.g., age) and second-order (e.g., the distribution of freely conferred deference) cues of prestige people use to infer competence within a valued domain. Fourth, we examine the evidence for and against the use of prestige bias in human adults and children. Finally, we point out limitations in the current literature and present new avenues for research on prestige bias.

Social learning and the evolution of prestige

Following H&GW, social rankFootnote 1 is defined as a hierarchy of rewards and/or displays in which individuals at the top enjoy privileges (e.g., preferential access to resources usually without resistance from other in-group members), are influential, and receive deference (i.e., manifestations of respect and submission to their wishes). High social rank is generally desirable and is positively associated with reproductive success in many societies (Betzig, 1988; Chagnon, 1988; J. Hill, 1984; Mealey, 1985; von Rueden et al., 2010; von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016).

According to H&GW, people use, not necessarily consciously, two distinct strategies to acquire and maintain high social rank: dominance and prestige (see also Cheng and Tracy, 2014; Cheng et al., 2013). The dominance strategy involves causing, or threatening to induce, costs to other individuals. If the use of this strategy is successful, this elicits fear in the other individuals, who defer and submit to the wishes of the dominant individual to avoid the potential costs. In contrast, the prestige strategy involves displaying competence in valued domains. If the use of this strategy is successful, this elicits admiration in other individuals, who defer and submit to the wishes of the prestigious individual in order to gain access to, and thus socially learn from, this individual, and to acquire other benefits such as private and public goods (Anderson and Kilduff, 2009a; Cheng and Tracy, 2014; Henrich, 2016; Price and Van Vugt, 2014; Von Rueden et al., 2008). Although the successful use of both strategies leads to receiving deference from other individuals, the key distinction between them is that dominant individuals receive coerced deference, while prestigious individuals receive voluntary (or ‘freely-conferred’) deference.

H&GW argue that the dominance strategy is phylogenetically ancestral to our species. Humans share this strategy with non-human primates (e.g., de Waal, 2000) and other social animals. In contrast, H&GW believe that the prestige strategy is probably unique to humans (but see Chapais, 2015; Garfield et al., 2018; Horner et al., 2010; Kendal et al., 2015 for evidence of prestige in non-human animals). This is because they assume that the evolution of prestige required high-fidelity social learning, which is arguably much more developed in humans than in other animals (Herrmann et al., 2007; Tennie et al., 2009). According to H&GW, the selective pressure that drove the evolution of prestige was the need to identify individuals within a group (i.e., not only kin) with “better-than-average” knowledge/skill from whom to learn (but see Barkow, 1989, 2014; Chapais, 2015 for alternative explanations for the evolution of prestige). Copying these individuals within domains such as medicinal plant knowledge or hunting techniques would have led to the acquisition of fitness enhancing knowledge/skills. However, directly inferring superior knowledge/skill is often difficult or costly. For instance, accurately assessing hunting skill is difficult when hunting highly dispersed large-size game in fluctuating environments, in which the variation in caloric returns does not depend exclusively on hunting skill (K. Hill and Kintigh, 2009). Under these circumstances, the use of less direct but more readily available proxies for identifying knowledgeable/skilful individuals is often adaptive. Such proxies include the amount of copying, attention and deference (i.e., costs paid in exchange for access to the model) that individuals freely receive. These prestige cues act as indirect cues of success to select models from whom to learn (Henrich, 2016).

H&GW also formulated a number of predictions derived from their theory of the evolution of prestige, and their theory has inspired a number of related predictions (Table 1, see also Table 2 for a full list of predictions derived from the present review).

Table 1 Predictions about prestige bias derived from the existing literature
Table 2 Predictions about prestige bias derived from the present review

Prestige, competence, and age

H&GW predict that knowledgeable/skilful individuals tend to acquire prestige (prediction (i) in Table 1). This prediction is based on their assumption that people defer to knowledgeable/skilful individuals to obtain preferential access to them, and consequently increase valuable social learning opportunities. H&GW support their prediction with ethnographies that show that prestige is associated with skill/knowledge in valued domains such as hunting (e.g., Holmberg, 1969, pp. 144–145; Lee, 1979, pp. 343–344), supernatural knowledge (e.g., Lee, 1979, pp. 343–344) or combat (e.g., Patton, 2000) in many foraging societies. More formally, this association between prestige and knowledge/skill has been supported by a recent study looking at more than 1000 ethnographical texts on leadership pertaining to 60 cultures contained in the Human Relations Area Files (HRAF) (Garfield et al. 2018). Moreover, H&GW assumed that prestige and skill/knowledge in valued domains is also positively correlated in modern post-industrial societies as for the case of male adolescents conferring prestige to peers with high athletic skills (Coleman, 1961, pp. 130–135). Similarly, H&GW predict that older individuals have higher prestige than younger ones, as older individuals have more experience in life and have had time to accumulate greater knowledge and refined skills (prediction (ii) in Table 1). They support this prediction with ethnographic evidence (Maxwell and Silverman, 1970; Simmons, 1945/1970). However, they do not mention modern post-industrial societies, which makes it difficult to know whether they assume that this correlation also holds in such societies.

Reyes-Garcia et al. (2008) tested these predictions with the Tsimane, an indigenous population from the Bolivian Amazon. Participants were asked to list all the important men in the village and say why they were important. Most of the nominations went to people with formal positions (72%), with the remaining nominations given due to the nominees’ personal attributes (12.5%), including being knowledgeable (2.5%). The initial measure of prestige was the number of nominations due to personal attributes. The measure of knowledge was ethnobotanical plant knowledge derived using cultural consensus analysis, i.e., the most common response to whether each of 15 plants had medicinal properties was considered the correct answer. The results did not support either of the two predictions: neither ethnobotanical medicinal knowledge nor age were significantly associated with their measure of prestige.

However, there are several potential explanations for this null finding. When prestige was measured by the raw number of nominations instead of only the nominations due to personal attributes, prestige was positively and significantly associated with having a formal position the previous year and with being born in the 1970s. This latter result might be due to the recent increase in life expectancy in this society, which may have disrupted the predicted association between prestige and age. Alternatively, Reyes-Garcia et al. suggest that the training provided by missionaries to young men to become competent bilinguals (Spanish/Tsimane) and spread the biblical message gave men born in the 1970s the skills to “navigate between the two worlds” (p. 280). This might explain their current higher prestige, as opposed to older men with superior ethnobotanical knowledge.

In the same population, Von Rueden et al. (2008) found a strong positive association between hunting ability and respect, i.e., the extent that a person is considered worthy of being admired, which can be seen as equivalent to H&GW’s prestige. This supports H&GW’s prediction of a positive correlation between knowledge/skill and prestige. Nevertheless, hunting is losing its importance in the studied village due to acculturation and has no effect on community-wide influence, measured as the influence of an individual in the resolution of a dispute during a community-wide meeting. Von Rueden et al. (2008) believe that the social transformation experienced by the Tsimane society might explain why hunting skill is correlated with respect but not with community-wide influence. Conferring respect to people with highly relevant skills in the past might be a residue of “old-fashioned” values, but these values do not predict community-wide influence because these skills are no longer useful. In contrast, currently relevant skills (e.g., formal education and Spanish fluency) within this society were significant predictors of community-wide influence (see predictions (i–ii) in Table 2). These authors also found an inverted-U-shaped relationship between age and each of four measures of high rank (success in a physical confrontation, getting one’s way in the group, community-wide influence and respect).

Henrich and Henrich (2010) found that, as predicted, prestige (being nominated as a yalewa vuku or ‘wise woman’) was positively associated with medicinal plant knowledge and age, while controlling for years of education in Fijian society. However, medicinal plant knowledge was assessed only indirectly as the number of nominations of women considered to know the most about medicinal plants (perceived knowledge), rather than direct assessments of actual knowledge. In a different study, Henrich and Broesch (2011) tested the same predictions in three Fijian villages, although also indirectly. The outcome variable there was the probability of an individual being selected as a model from whom to learn in each of three domains (fishing, growing yams and medicinal plants). Perceived success in another domain (cross-domain success) was used to measure prestige. The results supported the prediction of the positive association between prestige and knowledge/skill in different domains. Nevertheless, fishing and yam growing success were much better predictors of prestige (cross-domain success) than plant knowledge. Age was a significant predictor of being selected as a model from whom to learn in the domains of growing yams and medicinal plant knowledge in one of the villages and of fishing and medicinal plant knowledge in the entire sample. Henrich & Broesch reasoned that, because age is an indirect indicator of success, age is a much more relevant cue in domains such as medicinal plant knowledge in which there does not exist clear evidence of success (see prediction (iv) in Table 2). This potential explanation constitutes an important nuance to the original prediction (prediction (ii) in Table 1) by H&GW.

In the Hazda, hunter-gatherers in Tanzania, Stibbard-Hawkes et al. (2018) found that hunting prestige (measured using photo-rankings of hunters) positively predicted three measures of actual hunting success (aim with bow and arrow, pull strength and knowledge of animal vocalisations) but was uncorrelated with a fourth measure (visual acuity). These measures were collected by the researchers by implementing tasks such as an archery contest or using a digital bow pull scale. Similarly to Von Rueden et al. (2008), they found an inverted-U-shaped relationship between age and hunting prestige, and age and hunting success, which might be consequence of the decline of hunting skill after reaching peak skill at 40–55 years of age.

While suggestive, most of these studies do not provide clear evidence to effectively determine whether prestige is positively associated with knowledge/skill and/or age. The most important limitation is that all of the studies (except Stibbard-Hawkes et al., 2018 and von Rouden et al., 2008) used measures of prestige that cannot be easily equated to H&GW´s original definition of prestige. The study by Reyes-Garcia et al. (2008) measured the number of nominations of people who were considered “important” within Tsimane villages, which led participants to nominate people with formal leadership positions. Therefore, it confuses power and prestige. Similarly, Henrich and Broesch’s (2011) measure of prestige as cross-domain success is also problematic because prestige was initially considered to be domain-specific (H&GW, p. 170), although with some contradictions (“prestigious individuals are influential even beyond the domain of expertise”, H&GW, p. 184). Another limitation of some of these studies (Henrich and Broesch, 2011; Henrich and Henrich, 2010) is the use of perceived success instead of assessing success directly using objective measures as in the study by Stibbard-Hawkes et al., 2018. Although perceived success and prestige within a domain should be positively correlated given the fact that perceptions of success confer prestige, this does not serve to assess the adaptive role of prestige bias. In order to be adaptive in the first place, prestige should positively correlate with actual success (see prediction (iii) in Table 2). Using the most common answer as a measure of correct knowledge (Reyes-Garcia et al., 2008) is also problematic, as the most frequent answer might be wrong.

To sum up, it is difficult to convincingly say whether there exists an association between prestige and knowledge/skill, and prestige and age. Future research should use better defined, or more direct and relevant, measures of prestige. However, the reviewed studies are useful for refining the research agenda. For example, the inverted U-shaped relationship between prestige and age found by Von Rueden et al. (2008) and Stibbard-Hawkes et al., 2018 is more plausible than a linear relationship if prestige is correlated with skill and there is a decline in physical and cognitive abilities with older age (see H&GW p. 182, Supplementary Materials in Henrich and Henrich, 2010, p. 4, and prediction (vi) in Table 2). Moreover, older age might be an inefficient cue of prestige in societies where traditional skills/knowledge have decreased in importance and new skills have become more important for the community (e.g., speaking Spanish to deal with the outside world in Tsimane society, or computer skills in postindustrial technological society). Consequently, older age is a relevant cue of prestige only when the environment changes at a relatively slow rate, such that the correlation between prestige and knowledge is stable across cultural generations (Henrich, 2016). Rapid environmental change, however, disrupts the positive correlation between valuable knowledge and age as it leads to “a high rate of informational obsolescence” (Maxwell and Silverman, 1970, p. 388), which might even lead to prestige being conferred on to younger individuals (Spisak et al. 2014; see prediction (v) in Table 2).

Prestige cues

To select the best models from whom to learn, social learners can directly assess the competence of different models within a valued domain. For instance, seeing a model successfully hunting large prey or scoring several goals during a football match leads the social learner to infer that the model is successful within those domains. Of course, their success on one day might not be a reliable indicator of their general, sustained success within a domain, but the social learner can update this information when more information is provided (e.g., the results of the next hunting expedition or football match). Nevertheless, assessing competence through this procedure may be costly and noisy. Instead, social learners can use short-cuts either by making inferences from the appearance, personality, material possessions, etc. of the models, which we called first-order cues, or by relying on the behaviours of other social learners towards the models, which we called second-order cues. Both types of cues can be cheap ways to acquire valuable information, although they can also lead to useless or maladaptive behaviour.

First-order cues

The basis for the usefulness of first-order cues is that these short-cuts are usually positively associated with competence within valuable domains and/or confer prestige due to their close relationship with competence. For instance, the age of a model can be used to infer knowledge and skill as older individuals generally have more experience within a valued domain and, therefore, they can usually provide higher quality information. This cue is especially used and useful for young children as it is less cognitively demanding than other cues such as professed knowledge (Wood et al., 2012). Nevertheless, the usefulness of using age to socially learn depends on the social context and the pace of environmental change (see Section ‘Prestige, competence, and age').

Generosity has also been linked to prestige (prediction (iii) in Table 1) in experiments (e.g., Flynn et al. 2006; Halevy et al. 2012; Hardy and Van Vugt, 2006; Willer, 2009) and ethnographic observations (e.g., Konečná and Urlacher, 2017; Price, 2003; Radcliffe-Brown, 1964). Because prestigious individuals tend to be both competent and generous (Cheng and Tracy, 2014; Cheng et al. 2010; Henrich, 2016), at least towards members of their ingroup, generosity can be used as a proxy for competence. According to H&GW, this link is probably due to the fact that providing public goods is an excellent way to signal competence and, therefore, to receive further deference, which might be translated into fitness gains. Nevertheless, wealth is often inherited and, therefore, the relationship between being generous (e.g., providing private and public goods to other people) and being competent does not always hold. Moreover, generosity might be valued independently to competence (Bai, 2016) due to the direct benefits (e.g., private and public goods) provided to the group.

The self-assessment of one’s ability, which if positive can lead to high self-confidence, is often used as a proxy for competence. In fact, assertive and confident individuals tend to be granted higher social rank within groups (Anderson and Kilduff, 2009b). Although confidence is likely to be associated with competence in many circumstances (e.g., people who do not know about a topic cannot usually communicate their knowledge about it effectively), copying or conferring high social rank to overconfident models (i.e., models who overestimate their knowledge) might not be the best strategy, as other models will outperform them in reality. Similarly, pride displays, which generally occur after an achievement, are often proxies for competence and social rank (Tracy et al., 2013). Nevertheless, the actual relationship with competence might depend on the type of pride display (i.e., authentic vs. hubristic pride, see Cheng et al., 2010). Similarly, job titles within appreciated domains (e.g., doctor) and academic titles conferred by leading universities (e.g., Oxford or Harvard) also act as prestige cues (Burris, 2004; Dalmaso et al., 2012) in post-industrial societies. The same can be said for possessing wealth (Cheng and Tracy, 2013), prestige goods (Plourde, 2008) and wearing particular types of clothing such as suits (Bickman, 1971; DeWall and Maner, 2008; Maner et al., 2008). Nevertheless, it is necessary to emphasise that the identification of these cues as prestige cues has a subjective component because they depend on the values of the social learners and their social group. For instance, wearing a suit might be an inadequate cue of prestige within the punk rock scene, while having a multicolour Mohawk can be used as a prestige cue within this subculture. Likewise, a successful footballer might not be considered prestigious within a group of people who do not like football, as football is not a valuable domain for them. Consequently, research using first-order prestige cues should ensure that these cues are relevant for the participants.

In summary, although first-order cues have the clear advantage of being less cognitively demanding than assessing the success of a model directly, they are prone to be unreliable for two reasons. First, the relationship of some of these cues (e.g., age, or job titles) with competence depends on the task, context and/or rate of ecological and social environmental change. Second, some self-generated first-order cues (e.g., confidence) are open to cheating or deception, especially when prestigious individuals receive material or other benefits. Nevertheless, generosity is a self-generated first-order cue which is especially difficult to fake (Barclay, 2013).

Second-order cues

According to H&GW, individuals give freely conferred deference, pay more attention, seek proximity to, and copy, competent individuals (predictions (iv–vii) in Table 1). Consequently, social learners can use the behaviours of other individuals towards the models to select models from whom to learn. These second-order cues have the advantage of being considerably more difficult to fake and of being regularly updated.

The voluntary payment of costs (freely-conferred deference) in exchange for access to prestigious models (prediction (iv) in Table 1) is a central aspect of H&GW’s theory. H&GW argue that prestigious individuals are respected, receive unsolicited help, and are freed from some social obligations because social learners use these deference displays to try to grant themselves (not necessarily consciously) preferential access to prestigious individuals to gain valuable social learning opportunities. This preferential access to the models is important as many different skills contribute to the success of a model within a valued domain. H&GW gave the example of the potential factors involved in hunting success such as being good at making bows, aiming, tracking and approaching prey, as well as more indirect factors such as sleeping well or having an appropriate diet to maintain good eyesight. (Note, however, that if many people show deference to the same individual, the probability of gaining social learning opportunities would be small. Therefore, the adaptive value of paying deference to a highly deferred demonstrator might be minimal.)

Although the study was not conceived to test H&GW’s prediction, van der Vegt et al. (2006) found that students who self-perceived themselves as having low expertise carrying out a research project within four-person groups were more committed (measured with items like “I am very committed to maintain my relationship with X”) and provide more help (measured with items like “I assist X with difficult assignments, even when assistance is not directly requested”; “I help X when s/he is running behind in his/her work activities”) to individuals perceived as experts within their group. Consistent with H&GW’s prediction, this suggests that deference (helping and commitment) was provided towards perceived experts to incentivise their contribution to the group task and receive help from those experts. Importantly, this reciprocity in the exchange of help and commitment between members with high and low expertise was associated with higher performance in the task. This highlights the adaptive nature of this exchange, which has also been shown theoretically by Panchanathan (2010). Experimental evidence has also shown that high prestige individuals are paid more attention than low prestige individuals (Cheng et al., 2013; Dalmaso et al., 2014; Dalmaso et al., 2012; DeWall and Maner, 2008; Foulsham et al., 2010; Gerpott et al., 2018; Maner et al., 2008; Ratcliff et al., 2011) and that successful individuals tend to be copied (Atkisson et al. 2012; Burdett et al. 2016; McElreath et al., 2008; Mesoudi, 2008; Wood et al., 2013), which provide support for H&GW’s predictions about attention (prediction (v)) and copying (prediction (vii) in Table 1). This makes deferential, attentional and copying cues reliable cues to infer the prestige of an individual in a social group.

To our knowledge, no research has looked directly at the proximity-management strategies of social learners towards high prestige individuals. However, experimental research with WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrial, Rich and Democratic, see Henrich et al., 2010) samples shows that prestige positively predicts being liked (Brand and Mesoudi, 2018; Cheng et al., 2013), and preferred as a holiday companion, business partner, neighbour (Kruger and Fitzgerald, 2011) and long-term mate (Kruger and Fitzgerald, 2011; Snyder et al., 2008). This provides tentative support for H&GW’s prediction on proximity-management towards prestigious individuals (prediction (vi) in Table 1). However, it is possible that this proximity-management might be motivated by other concerns (e.g., coalitional support) than social learning, which is necessary for supporting H&GW’s model. Furthermore, the ethnographical record shows little support for the assumption (prediction (viii) in Table 1) that underpins all these predictions, that is the preferential copying of what prestigious individuals do (Garfield et al., 2018). Nevertheless, this absence of evidence might be motivated by the lack of interest of earlier anthropologists in social learning.

Prestige-biased social learning

Prestige and success biases

H&GW predict that prestigious individuals are preferentially copied, which is known as prestige bias (prediction (viii) in Table 1). Importantly, this prediction should only hold when both individual learning and the direct assessment of knowledge/skill of a model are costly or difficult. When the acquisition of knowledge/skill through individual learning is relatively cheap, the use of prestige bias (or social learning in general) is less useful. Similarly, when information about the success of individuals is directly available, people should use this information to select models (success bias) rather than prestige (prediction (ix) in Table 1).

Consequently, Atkisson et al. (2012) compared prestige and success biases in the laboratory. Participants played a computer-based task in which they designed virtual arrowheads over a series of trials (see Mesoudi, 2008; Mesoudi and O’Brien, 2008). Participants could improve their arrowhead by either individual or social learning. There were three hunting seasons. Prestige information (time spent by each of the participants looking at the arrowheads designed by each of the four other participants) was provided to participants throughout the experiment. Success information (score of the four different individuals who used different arrowheads) was only provided in season three. The results supported the prediction about the use of prestige bias: prestige information increased the likelihood of an arrowhead being copied during seasons one and two compared to the other arrowheads. In contrast, the results did not support the prediction about the replacement of prestige bias by success bias when both success and prestige information are available: prestige and success cues were used similarly during season three, even though prestige was not correlated with success in the experiment.

This experiment provides convincing evidence for prestige bias. Nevertheless, it is not clear why participants used prestige and success cues in a similar way in season three. Atkisson et al. (2012) argue that participants use prestige cues to socially learn in their everyday life and this extends to their behaviour in the laboratory. It is also possible that success needs to be observed over longer periods of time to replace prestige bias. Alternatively, perhaps participants were over-trained to use prestige cues in seasons one to two and carried this into season three. A replication adding a second condition, in which success cues are provided during seasons one to two, might clarify this. We might expect participants to learn more easily that success cues are superior to prestige cues in this case. Finally, it would be interesting to systematically manipulate the correlation between prestige and success, to see whether prestige cues are only used when prestige is positively correlated with success (see prediction (vii) in Table 2).

Variation in the use of prestige bias with experience and age

Exploratory analyses by Atkisson et al. (2012) showed that the use of prestige and success biases was greater when participants performed badly in the previous three trials. Another circumstance that might prompt greater use of these biases is the lack of previous experience within a domain. Consequently, the lower level of experience of younger people compared to older people might make younger people more prone to copy prestigious/successful individuals (prediction (x) in Table 1).

Little et al. (2015) tested this prediction in the domain of mate choice. In an initial experiment, female participants rated the attractiveness of young, artificially-created male faces paired with female faces with different degrees of prestige, which was manipulated by presenting a numeric score of popularity. As predicted (prediction (viii) in Table 1), model popularity positively predicted ratings of male face attractiveness. Moreover, older participants were less likely to be influenced by the popularity of the models than younger participants, consistent with the prediction that prestige bias should vary with experience/age. However, a limitation of this experiment is that the male faces being rated were all very young. Given evidence that women are less attracted to men who are considerably younger than themselves than men who are of similar age or older (Buss, 1989; Buunk et al. 2001; Schwarz and Hassebrauck, 2012), this may explain the age effect rather than experience. This was addressed by Little et al. in a subsequent experiment, which used real photographs and in which three age groups were used for both participants (16–25, 26–32, 32–61) and stimuli (18–25, 26–32, 32–40). Again as predicted, model popularity positively predicted the ratings of attractiveness for the younger group (16–25 years old) but not for the older groups of participants (25–32, 32–61).

Nevertheless, the problem remained: a considerable number of participants in the oldest group (M = 41.6, SD = 8.1) were still rating exclusively much younger male faces than themselves. Consequently, the interpretation of the findings requires similar caution. Moreover, the effect size was more than double for the interaction between age of the face and participants’ age (ηp2 = 0.10) than for the interaction between model popularity and participants’ age (ηp2 = 0.04). This suggests that rather than experience-dependent prestige bias, Little et al.’s findings can be explained by the congruency between age of faces and age of participants. Further experiments should ensure that the stimuli are maximally relevant for the participants.

Contrary to Little et al.’s prediction, the two-stage social learning model (Henrich and Broesch, 2011; Henrich and Henrich, 2010; Kline et al., 2013) predicts a greater use of prestige bias with age/experience. As there exists a trade-off between the access costs to different models and the fitness-enhancing information that can be acquired from the models, social learners should first learn from low access cost models (e.g., relatives, neighbours, friends) and later further improve their knowledge/skill by copying prestigious and/or successful models. This updating process would be more noticeable when there is large variation in knowledge/skill within a given domain so that social learners would benefit more from copying high competence models using success or prestige cues. When the variation is small, most social learners would not copy successful/prestigious models because much of the information they can learn from them is shared by almost everyone in the social group and, therefore, they can learn fitness-enhancing knowledge/skills from low access cost models instead (Henrich and Henrich, 2010; see prediction (ix) in Table 2). Mathematical models that include a combination of vertical and oblique transmission have shown that, if some members of each generation use model-based biases (e.g., prestige bias, success bias), fitness-enhancing knowledge/skills will spread in a population over generations, leading to the emergence of cultural adaptation (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Henrich, 2004; Powell et al., 2009). This gives plausibility to the adaptive value of the two-stage social learning model.

This pattern seems to be the case in the transmission of adaptive food taboos related to pregnancy and breastfeeding in Fijian villages. According to Henrich and Henrich (2010), the pattern of transmission is mainly from older (i.e., mothers, grand-mothers, mothers-in-law) to younger (i.e., daughters, granddaughter, daughters-in-law) female relatives. Nevertheless, a substantial minority (almost 25%) of participants in this study reported to have learnt the taboos from the yalewa vuku (wise women, who were equated to prestigious individuals), or the elders (almost 33%). Although this gives tentative support for the emergence of cultural adaptation through a combination of vertical and oblique transmission, it is not clear whether prestige-biased transmission was greater at an older than a younger age. The acquisition of relevant skills for wild honey collecting among male Jenu Kuruba in South India also seems to follow the two-stage social learning model. According to Demps et al. (2012), most honey collecting knowledge/skill is acquired in this population at younger ages: most people reported to learn tree climbing at six to nine years old, making a smoky torch at 10–15 years old, and cutting honey combs at 16–21 years old. Importantly, most of the knowledge/skills were learnt from relatives (fathers, brothers, and elder kin) but learning from successful individuals and co-workers became more important with age. Nevertheless, the two-stage social learning model is not specific to the use of prestige cues (see Lucas et al., 2016 for further discussion and experimental evidence).

Prestige bias and overimitation

If prestige bias is especially likely to be used when success is difficult to directly assess (see Section ‘Social learning and the evolution of prestige'), people should copy irrelevant actions carried out by prestigious individuals when the link between each action and success in the task is not clear for them. Experiments on overimitation, the tendency to copy irrelevant actions to obtain a reward, have looked at whether younger children overimitate high social rank models. McGuigan (2013) conducted an experiment in which five year olds viewed videos with one of four models with high or low social rank performing irrelevant actions (e.g., removing a bolt) and relevant actions (e.g., extracting the sticker with the tool) to obtain a sticker from a transparent box. The high social rank models were the participants’ head and class teacher. The low social rank models were a familiar model (a researcher who had carried out research with the children during the previous week) and an unfamiliar model (a totally unacquainted individual for the children). As predicted, children copied irrelevant actions significantly more when they came from the high social rank models than from the low social rank models. Nevertheless, pairwise comparisons only found significant differences between the head teacher and both low rank models. Moreover, the copying of relevant actions was not affected by models’ rank. Alternative factors, rather than prestige, might account for the results, as the high rank models were also more familiar to the children and held a position of authority over them.

In contrast, Chudek et al. (2016) obtained findings that cast doubt on importance of model-based biases in overimitation. Children aged two to seven years viewed videos with two female adult models trying to obtain stickers from a puzzle box. In one condition, the models had low or high prestige, which was manipulated by showing two individuals carefully looking at the actions performed by one model (high prestige) while ignoring the actions of the other model (low prestige). In another condition, the models had high or low success, which was manipulated by the models either saying they obtained five (high success) or zero (low success) stickers. While overimitation generally increased with age, there was no selectivity at any age: the children were equally likely to overimitate successful and unsuccessful, and prestigious and non-prestigious, models. Importantly, these negative results were found using models who were not familiar to the children and who did not have a position of authority over them, suggesting that these factors might have driven the selective overimitation in McGuigan’s (2013) experiment.

Prestige bias beyond the domain of prestige

H&GW predict that prestigious individuals are influential beyond their domain of expertise (prediction (xi) in Table 1). This prediction is based on the following assumption: as it is difficult or costly to identify the factors that make someone successful within a valued domain, natural selection should have favoured a general-copying bias towards the prestigious, in the hope that at least one of the many characteristics that are copied are causally related to success. For instance, many factors might lead to being a successful hunter (tracking skill, ability and materials use to make bows, sleeping well, etc.) and, consequently, a general copying bias of all of these traits associated with the prestigious might be adaptive, at the occasional cost of copying neutral or maladaptive traits (e.g., wearing a magic charm, or being tattooed). This general social learning bias towards prestigious individuals might help to explain why the opinions of prestigious individuals within a given domain (e.g., acting or singing in Western society or hunting in a foraging society) are influential in other domains (e.g., Arnocky et al. 2018; Jackson and Darrow, 2005; Lee, 1979, p. 343; Radcliffe-Brown, 1964, p. 64; Smith and Bird, 2000). Another possibility not considered by H&GW is that there exist domain-general traits that are likely to lead to success in multiple domains, such as having an inner locus of control, intrinsic motivation, general intelligence/IQ, or being perseverant and self-disciplined. These domain-general traits might explain in part the success of some celebrities such as Will Smith (acting, rapping) or Arnold Schwarzenegger (bodybuilding, acting, politics) across multiple domains (although being famous itself may also have helped them to achieve success in other domains). Nevertheless, a cross-domain social learning bias towards the prestigious might sometimes cause the acquisition of maladaptive information and, consequently, it should be expected that the influence of prestigious individuals is larger within their domain of prestige.

To test this, Chudek et al. (2012) conducted two studies with three to four years olds. The manipulation of prestige involved varying the degree of attention that two different models received from other individuals. The models then showed a preference for one of two foods, drinks, artefacts or labels for novel objects. Afterwards, children’s preferences for the same item pairs were tested. The results for the first study supported prestige-biased social learning (prediction (viii) in Table 1), as children were more likely to share the preferences of the prestigious model over the non-prestigious model. The second study in which the models displayed preferences only in one of two domains (food or artefacts) also provided support for the prestige-biased social learning hypotheses, but confined within the domain of prestige. That is, children followed the preference of the prestigious model only for the specific domain in which they saw that model exhibit preferences. Consequently, this study did not provide support for H&GW’s prediction of cross-domain prestige-biased social learning. Future studies might benefit from studying more directly the two aforementioned mechanisms that favour cross-domain prestige bias, as they make specific and as-yet untested predictions. From the first mechanism (general copying bias) it follows that cross-domain prestige bias should occur when cues are noisy within the tested domain (see prediction (x) in Table 2). From the second mechanism (cross-domain general ability) it follows that cross-domain prestige bias should occur when there are domain-general traits like IQ that make people successful across domains (see prediction (xi) in Table 2).

Comparisons between prestige and content biases

Although theoretically prestige biased social learning is generally adaptive, so are other social learning biases, and it is instructive to compare people’s use of prestige bias relative to other biases. Acerbi and Tehrani (2018) conducted two studies that compare the strength of content and prestige in cultural transmission. Content biases occur when certain types of material are preferentially transmitted, in contrast to model-based biases such as prestige bias where characteristics of the model are used. Acerbi and Tehrani (2018) chose the topic of quotations because it is a domain in which both content (the message of the quote) and prestige (e.g., frequent misattribution of quotes to famous people) are relevant. First, participants rated a series of unattributed quotes for their likeability, i.e., content. Then, they tested whether quotes associated with famous (i.e., prestigious) individuals (e.g., Vincent Van Gogh) were more liked than the same quotes associated with non-famous individuals invented by the researchers (e.g., Winston Perkins). The results showed no statistical difference in liking ratings between quotes associated with the high and low prestige individuals. Instead, liking was significantly predicted by the original likeability ratings of the quotes when unattributed. This suggests that the content of the quotations is more important than the attributed authorship of the quotation.

Acerbi and Tehrani argue that the lack of evidence for prestige bias in their study might have been because the task did not require any kind of expertise. Furthermore, participants did not obtain higher or lower rewards for preferring some quotations over others. Consequently, the use of prestige bias was not useful here as participants could easily assess the likability of the quotes by themselves without any cost (see prediction (viii) in Table 2). Alternatively, people might have assumed that the “low prestige” names were “high prestige” too but that they had not heard of them before.

As was argued in section ‘Variation in the use of prestige bias with experience and age', the usefulness of prestige bias (and social learning in general) should depend on people’s expertise. When people lack experience within a given domain, they should benefit more from using prestige cues to select models from whom to learn (prediction (ix) in Table 1). However, the opposite can also be true, i.e., prestige bias is used more by those with high expertise, when the domain is more relevant for experts than non-experts, and when the task is more difficult for experts than non-experts. This is what Verpooten and Dewitte (2017) found for the appreciation of modern art. Like Acerbi and Tehrani, they used a subjective task in which there was no objective correct or incorrect answer. Laypeople and art experts were shown portraits of female faces, which varied in attractiveness (moderate vs. high attractiveness). In one condition, participants were shown these pictures without any additional information. In another condition, participants were told that the pictures belonged to a collection of a prestigious museum in New York (MoMA). As predicted, laypeople’s appreciation was guided by the content of the pictures, showing more appreciation for the highly attractive faces than the moderately attractive faces with little influence of the prestige manipulation. As was also predicted, experts showed more appreciation for the pictures associated with the prestigious museum. This relationship was mediated by admiration towards the artists, which is consistent with H&GW’s theory (see Section ‘Social learning and the evolution of prestige'). Surprisingly, they also found that experts appreciated more the moderately attractive faces than the highly attractive faces. In another study, Verpooten (2018) used real artworks from MoMA, which depicted animate (e.g., pig) or inanimate (e.g., chair) objects. According to evolutionary psychologists (New et al., 2007), people have an evolved preference for animate over inanimate objects, which the authors predicted would guide the behaviour of non-experts. Consistent with this, there was an overall preference for animate over inanimate objects. Again, however, this was moderated by expertise. The greater the expertise of the participants, the less the participants appreciated the animate over the inanimate artworks, to the extent the preferences reversed in the top experts. Verpooten and Dewitte (2017), inspired by previous work (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Prum, 2013), argue that the experts’ deviations in artwork preferences from evolved aesthetic preferences might be due to a runaway process analogous to a runway sexual selection in which the trait (here, artworks) coevolves with preferences within a population of art experts.

Although the research covered in this section is interesting with respect to the general phenomenon of prestige, it is not clear how the likability of quotes or artworks is related to social learning. Although a greater appreciation of a cultural item probably leads to the greater transmissibility of that item, this may not always be the case. Disgust-inducing information, for instance, is not particularly liked, yet several studies have shown that it has a transmission advantage (Eriksson and Coultas, 2014; Heath et al., 2001; Miton et al., 2015), at least, in WEIRD samples (Eriksson et al., 2016). For instance, an offensive comment on Twitter (e.g., one that is overtly racist) or Marcel Duchamp’s Fountain might be disgusting and be disliked by many people, but this in itself could contribute to these items’ greater transmission by commenting on them or drawing attention to them. Future studies should test the differential effects of the content of the information and the prestige associated to the source by designing experiments that measure transmission in a more direct way using, for instance, experimental paradigms such as transmission chains (Bartlett, 1932; Mesoudi, 2007) and choose-to-transmit (Eriksson and Coultas, 2014; Heath et al., 2001; Stubbersfield et al., 2014) and choose-to-receive (Eriksson and Coultas, 2014; Stubbersfield et al., 2014) methods.

Final remarks and future directions

In this article, we have reviewed the evidence amassed for the adaptiveness and use of prestige bias in human adults and children since the publication of H&GW’s influential paper and suggested new predictions and research questions (see Table 2). Although H&GW predicted that prestige is positively correlated with both skill/knowledge within valued domains and age (predictions (i–ii) in Table 1), the specific tests of these claims inspired by H&GW’s theory suggest a more complex picture. Above all, the positive association between prestige and both knowledge/skill and age heavily depends on the stability of the social and ecological environment. When there is rapid social change, the skills/knowledge that were important and valuable in the past might not be any longer. Nevertheless, people might still confer prestige according to old-fashioned values, which would disrupt the predicted positive correlation between prestige and knowledge/skill. Alternatively, people might confer prestige according to new values that confer importance to the skills that are relevant nowadays. In this case, if the researchers measure the correlation between prestige and a particular domain of knowledge/skill that has lost its importance within a society, the predicted positive association between prestige and knowledge/skill would be disrupted (see predictions (i–iii) in Table 2). The same can be said about the predicted positive correlation between prestige and age. Age is a good cue of knowledge/skill when the social and ecological environment is relatively stable and life expectancy does not go much beyond an age at which the degradation of cognitive and physical skills starts. When there is rapid environmental change, older age might be an inadequate cue to select models from whom to learn and sometimes even younger models might be preferred. Moreover, the degradation of cognitive and physical skills with age might moderate the positive association between age and knowledge/skill and favour a plateau or a decline at older age. Future research should explore how social and ecological change and the age-dependency in skill/knowledge within different domains affects the association between prestige, knowledge/skill and age (see predictions (v–vi) in Table 2).

The evidence reviewed in this article provides mixed support for the use of prestige-biased social learning in both human adults and children. However, few studies have examined this and further research is needed to clarify which factors lead to variation in the use of prestige-biased social learning. The difficulty of the task, the relevance of the domain for the individuals and the benefits and costs associated with the task seem to be important factors influencing the use of prestige-biased social learning (see prediction (viii) in Table 2). In general, easy tasks, tasks that are not relevant for participants and tasks that do not provide incentives to perform well or avoid costs (e.g., monetary rewards or costs) seem not to stimulate the use of prestige-biased social learning (Acerbi and Tehrani, 2018). Other factors taken into account in the literature, such as experience and age (Little et al., 2015) seem to be important when they affect task difficulty, the relevance of the domain for the participants and potential gains or costs of the task for the participants. For instance, expertise leads to a greater use of prestige-biased social learning when the task is more relevant for the experts but the task is still difficult for them (Verpooten and Dewitte, 2017). Similarly, younger individuals use more prestige-biased social learning than older individuals when the task is more relevant for them (Little et al., 2015). Moreover, when there is little variation in knowledge/skill in a group, it is more adaptive to learn from low access cost models than from costly prestigious models (Henrich and Henrich, 2010; see prediction (ix) in Table 2).

Another factor that influences the use of prestige-biased social learning is the availability of alternative social learning biases, e.g., success or content biases. When success information is provided, this information should be preferentially used over prestige information (prediction (ix) in Table 1). However, this was not found in the sole experiment comparing prestige with success bias (Atkisson et al., 2012), although this is a single study. Both direct and conceptual replications are needed to gain confidence in this result. Content bias was stronger than prestige bias in another study (Acerbi and Tehrani, 2018), but this might depend on the domain and the factors mentioned above (i.e., task difficulty, relevance for the individual, and benefits and costs associated with the task). Variation in some of these factors (e.g., the relevance for the participants) might lead some participants (e.g., non-experts) to make use of content biases, while other participants (e.g., experts) to employ prestige-biased social learning (Verpooten and Dewitte, 2017).

It is also possible that prestige biased social learning has different effects on different measures of influence, e.g., recall, likability, behavioural influence, willingness to transmit and receive information. To the extent these measures of influence affect task difficulty, relevance for the participants or the benefits/costs associated with tasks, it seems plausible that the different measures would be a source of variation in the use of prestige and other social learning biases. For instance, although one recent study found that anti-vaccination messages are not better transmitted per se, exploratory analyses showed that when anti-vaccination messages are provided by doctors (i.e., a prestigious source within a relevant domain) these types of messages are especially powerful in influencing people’s vaccination-related decisions (Jiménez et al., 2018). Similarly, although people might be able to appreciate the content of certain pieces of information (e.g., quotes, news, artworks, etc.) independent of the prestige of the source of the information, they might be more influenced by prestige cues when they want to achieve influence over other people’s behaviour (e.g., by quoting a prestigious source of information), get personal or social benefits (e.g., choosing artworks to be displayed in their own town) or they have to decide whether to learn more about a topic or transmit the information about the topic to other people. Therefore, research on prestige-biased social learning might benefit from comparing the influence of prestige cues on different types of outcomes.

Although H&GW predicted that prestige-biased social learning is cross-domain such that prestige in one domain bleeds across to other domains (prediction (xi) in Table 1), the only experiment testing this hypothesis found that prestige-biased social learning is stronger within domains (Chudek et al., 2012). Nevertheless, anecdotal evidence (e.g., the influence of the opinions of celebrities in domains in which they are not experts) suggests that cross-domain prestige-biased social learning occurs to some extent. In small-scale societies, it is likely that prestigious individuals are relatively skilful/knowledgeable across multiple domains and, consequently, a cross-domain prestige bias would be adaptive (Acerbi, 2016), albeit leading to the occasional acquisition of irrelevant or even maladaptive information. However, the risks of cross-domain prestige-biased social learning seem considerably higher in the digital era in which, for example, young people in developing countries might be more influenced by American pop singers, Hollywood celebrities or leaders of terrorist groups than by the nearby adults who have relevant skills for their environment (Barkow et al., 2012). Consequently, studying to what extent the digital media have subverted the adaptive role of prestige-biased social learning seems a productive new avenue for research on prestige and social learning (Acerbi, 2016; Barkow et al., 2012).

H&GW predicted that information provided by prestigious individuals is more memorable, but this question has not been studied yet. If H&GW are correct and prestige-based hierarchies evolved to select fitness-enhancing models from whom to learn, prestige-biased social learning should strongly affect human memory. However, it is possible that the effects of prestige on memory are just the consequence of the preferential attention that prestigious individuals receive (see Section ‘Second-order cues'). Therefore, studies exploring how prestige cues affect memory should look at how differential attention towards prestigious and non-prestigious individuals affects memorability (e.g., by using eye-tracking).

In conclusion, H&GW’s theory of the evolution of prestige has generated a great deal of research and this research has stimulated new research questions and predictions. Although the evidence reviewed here suggest that prestige-bias social learning is employed in at least some contexts, further research will need to determine the precise circumstances in which people use prestige cues to learn socially, and when the use of these cues is adaptive.